Changes in the affective state and in the emotional attribution during estradiol fluctuations of the ovarian cycle in university students

Changes in the affective state and in the emotional attribution during estradiol fluctuations of the ovarian cycle in university students

Main Article Content

María Isabel Gómez León

Abstract

The purpose of this research was to analyze if some type of relation existed between the fluctuations of estradiol of the ovarian cycle, two variables of the affectivity, the dysthymia and the euthymia, and the type of emotional attributions, positive, negative or neutral, realized to semantic and facial stimuli. The hormonal levels of 17-ß estradiol have measured up in saliva, across kits ELISA (Enzyme-Linked Immuno Sorbent Assay) in 64 women, aged 18-19 years. The subjects realized the Inventory of Depressive State/Characteristic (IDER) and two tests of emotional attribution, for verbal and facial stimuli, in two moments of the cycle, when the levels of estradiol were high and when these were lower. The studied women obtained significantly top punctuations in the items of eutimia state and in the number of positive attributions realized, with both facial and semantic stimuli, when they were during the more high beak of estradiol, that when the levels of estradiol were low. The results suggest that the estradiol might be one of the influential factors in the modulation and regulation of the positive affective state. This research seeks to give a new approach to strengthen psychological resilience and promote mental health.

References

Albert, K., Pruessner, J. y Newhouse, P. (2015). Estradiol Levels Modulate Brain Activity and
Negative Responses to Psychosocial Stress across the Menstrual Cycle.
Psychoneuroendocrinology, 59, 14–24.

Armbruster, D., Grage, T., Kirschbaum, C. y Strobel, A. (2018). Processing emotions: Effects of
menstrual cycle phase and premenstrual symptoms on the startle reflex, facial EMG and
heart rate. Behav Brain Res. 1(351), 178-187.

Bayer, J., Schultz, H., Gamer, M. y Sommer, T. (2014). Menstrual-cycle dependent fluctuations in
ovarian hormones affect emotional memory. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory,
110, 55-63.

Brockmeyer, T., Kulessa, D., Hautzinger, M., Bents, H. y Backenstrass, M. (2015). Moodincongruent
processing during the recall of a sad life event predicts the course and
severity of depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 187, 91–96.

Bush, K.A., Inman, C.S., Hamann, S., Kilts, C.D. y James, G.A. (2017). Distributed Neural
Processing Predictors of Multi-dimensional Properties of Affect. Frontiers in Human
Neuroscience, 11, 459.

da Costa, S. y Páez, D. (2015). Afectividad inducida e impacto en la creatividad, crecimiento
personal despue?s del cambio y ajuste percibido al narrar una experiencia emocional
intensa. Anales de Psicología, 31(2), 716-724.

Del Río, J.P., Alliende, M.I., Molina, N., Serrano, F.G., Molina, S. y Vigil, P. (2018). Steroid
Hormones and Their Action in Women’s Brains: The Importance of Hormonal Balance.
Frontiers in Public Health, 6, 141.

Diekhof, E.K. y Ratnayake, M. (2016). Menstrual cycle phase modulates reward sensitivity and
performance monitoring in young women: Preliminary fMRI evidence.
Neuropsychologia, 84, 70-80.

Dreher, J.C., Schmidt, P.J., Kohn, P., Furman, D., Rubinow, D. y Berman, K.F. (2007). Menstrual
cycle phase modulates reward-related neural function in women. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(7), 2465–2470.

Fernández, A., Díez, E., Alonso, M.A. y Beato, M.S. (2004). Free-association norms for the
Spanish names of the Snodgrass and Vanderwart pictures. Behavior Research Methods,
Instruments, & Computers, 36, 577-583.

Flöter A.J., Nathorst-Böös, J., Carlström, K. y von Schoultz, B. (2002). Addition of testosterone to
estrogen replacement therapy in oophorectomized women: effects on sexuality and wellbeing,
Climacteric, 5(4), 357-365.

Gordon, J.L., Rubinow, D.R., Eisenlohr-Moul, T.A., Xia, K., Schmidt, P.J. y Girdler, S.S. (2018).
Efficacy of Transdermal Estradiol and Micronized Progesterone in the Prevention of
Depressive Symptoms in the Menopause TransitionA Randomized Clinical Trial. JAMA
Psychiatry, 75(2), 149–157.

Graham, B.M., Denson, T.F., Barnett, J., Calderwood, C. y Grisham, J.R. (2018). Sex Hormones
Are Associated With Rumination and Interact With Emotion Regulation Strategy Choice
to Predict Negative Affect in Women Following a Sad Mood Induction. Frontiers in
Psychology, 9, 937.

Grupe, D.W., Schaefer, S.M., Lapate, R.C., Schoen, A.J., Gresham, L.K., Mumford, J.A. y
Davidson, R.J. (2018). Behavioral and neural indices of affective coloring for neutral
social stimuli. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 13(3), 310–320.

Herrera, A.Y., Hodis, H.N., Mack, W.J. y Mather, M. (2017) Estradiol Therapy After Menopause
Mitigates Effects of Stress on Cortisol and Working Memory. The Journal of Clinical
Endocrinology y Metabolism. J Clin Endocrinol, 102(12),4457-4466.

Karlsson, S., Henningsson, S., Hovey, D., Zettergren, A., Jonsson, L., Cortes, D.S. y Westberg, L.
(2016). Social memory associated with estrogen receptor polymorphisms in women.
Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(6), 877–883.

Kiesner, J., Mendle, J., Eisenlohr-Moul, T.A. y Pastore, M. (2016). Cyclical symptom change
across the menstrual cycle. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 4, 882–894.

Le Moult, J. y Gotlib, I.H. (2018). Depression: A cognitive perspective. Clinical Psychology
Review, (17), 30391-30394.

Li, S.H. y Graham, B.M. (2017). Why are women so vulnerable to anxiety, trauma- and stressrelated
disorders? The potential role of sex hormones. Lancet Psychiatry, 4,73–82.

Lorenz, T.K., Gesselman, A.N. y Vitzthum, V.J. (2017). Variance in Mood Symptoms Across
Menstrual Cycles: Implications for Premenstrual Dysphoric Disorder. Women’s
Reproductive Health (Philadelphia, Pa.), 4(2), 77–88.

Mare?ková, K., Perrin, J.S., Nawaz Khan, I., Lawrence, C., Dickie, E. y McQuiggan, D.A. (2014).
Hormonal contraceptives, menstrual cycle and brain response to faces. Social Cognitive
and Affective Neuroscience, 9(2), 191–200.

Mulligan, E.M., Nelson, B.D., Infantolino, Z.P., Luking, K.R., Sharma, R. y Hajcak, G. (2018).
Effects of menstrual cycle phase on electrocortical response to reward and depressive
symptoms in women. Psychophysiology, 16 PÁG

Natale, V., Albertazzi, P. y Cangini, A. (2003). The Effects of Menstrual Cycle on Dreaming.
Biological Rhythm Research, 34(3), 295-303.

Okely, J.A., Weiss, A. y Gale, C.R. (2017). The interaction between stress and positive affect in
predicting mortality. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 100, 53–60.

Pletzer, B., Kronbichler, M., Aichhorn, M., Bergmann, J., Ladurner, G. y Kerschbaum, H.H.
(2010). Menstrual cycle and hormonal contraceptive use modulate human brain
structure. Brain Res. 12(1348), 55-62.

Preece, D., Becerra, R., Robinson, K., Dandy, J. y Allan, A. (2018). Measuring emotion regulation
ability across negative and positive emotions: The Perth Emotion Regulation
Competency Inventory (PERCI). Personality and Individual Differences, 135, 229-241.

Rubinow, D. R., Smith, M.J., Schenkel, L.A., Schmidt, P.J. y Dancer, K. (2007). Facial Emotion
Discrimination Across the Menstrual Cycle in Women with Premenstrual Dysphoric
Disorder (PMDD) and Controls. Journal of Affective Disorders, 104(1-3), 37–44.

Schiller, D. y Delgado, M.R. (2010). Overlapping neural systems mediating extinction, reversal
and regulation of fear. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 268–276.

Schwarz, J., Rief, W., Radkovsky, A., Berking, M. y Kleinstauber, M. (2017). Negative affect as
mediator between emotion regulation and medically unexplained symptoms. Journal of
Psychosomatic Research, 101,114-121.

Sundström-Poromaa (2018). The Menstrual Cycle Influences Emotion but Has Limited Effect on
Cognitive Function. Vitam Horm. 107, 349-376.

Spielberger, C.D., Agudelo, D. y Buela-Casal, G. (2008). Inventario de Depresión Estado/Rasgo
(IDER). Madrid: TEA Ediciones.

Spielberger, C.D., Ritterband, L., Reheiser, E. y Brunner, T. (2003). The nature and measurement
of depression. Revista Internacional de Psicología Clínica y de la Salud/International
Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology, 3, 209-234. Recuperado de
http://www.redalyc.org/pdf/337/33730201.pdf

Spindler, H., Denollet, J., Kruse, C. y Pedersen, S.S. (2009). Positive affect and negative affect
correlate differently with distress and health-related quality of life in patients with
cardiac conditions: Validation of the Danish Global Mood Scale. Journal of
Psychosomatic Research, 67(1), 57-65.

van Wingen, G., van Broekhoven, F., Verkes R.J., Petersson, K.M., Bäckström, T. y Buitelaar, J.
(2008). Progesterone selectively increases amygdala reactivity in women. Mol.
Psychiatry, VOL/NUM, 13325–13333.

Wang, X., Wang, Y., Ma, L., Zhang, R., De, Y., Yang, X. y Wu, Q. (2015). Development of an
improved competitive ELISA based on a monoclonal antibody against
lipopolysaccharide for the detection of bovine brucellosis. BMC Veterinary Research,
11, 118.

Yen, J.Y., Wang, P.W., Su, C.H., Liu, T.L., Long, C.Y. y Ko, C.H. (2018). Estrogen levels,
emotion regulation, and emotional symptoms of women with premenstrual dysphoric
disorder: The moderating effect of estrogen receptor 1? polymorphism. Prog
Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2(82), 216-223.

Young, A., Perrett, D.I., Calder, A., Sprengelmeyer, R.H. y Ekman, P.(2002). Facial expressions
of emotion: Stimuli and Test (FEEST).

Zhang, Y. y Han, B. (2016). Positive affect and mortality risk in older adults: A meta-analysis.
Psych J. Jun, 5(2):125-138.