What it is may not be what it seems. About the high capacities

What it is may not be what it seems. About the high capacities

Main Article Content

Silvia Mayoral Rodríguez
Carme Timoneda Gallart
Frederic Pérez Álvarez

Abstract

This study examines the DN-CAS versus WISC to assess gifted children with learning difficulties or conduct disorders (twiceexceptional children). 114 gifted children, from 10 years to 14 years old (female 56%), 54 with learning difficulties, 60 with conduct disorder, were assessed on DN-CAS (PASS) and WISC. Factor analysis showed four PASS factors and one WISC factor. WISC loaded mainly on simultaneous. No other PASS processing was linked with WISC. Correlations showed weak or moderate correlation between CAS and WISC. Simultaneous is the less correlated process and therefore we can infer that it woks more independently. Descriptive statistics showed that simultaneous was the higher score and planning the lower score. On analyzing clusters of PASS processes higher that 115 (+1SD) and 130 (+2SD), simultaneous appears as the most frequently involved. Finally, a particular pattern (”N” pattern) is found as a marker of emotional dysfunction linked to planning. These findings demonstrate a four-factor solution for D-N CAS among the gifted children tested. Only simultaneous appears linked to WISC. A good or excellent simultaneous appears linked to gifted children. The DN-CAS seems to offer an advantageous tool to assess gifted children, mainly in the case of underachievers or disturbed behavior, given that DN-CAS assesses planning, closely related to creativity and metacognition.

References

Artola, T., Ancillo, I., Barraca, J.,y Mosteiro, P. (2010). PIC-N. Prueba de Imaginación Creativa para Niños.[Creative Imagination Test for Children]. Madrid: TEA Ediciones.

Artola, T., Barraca, J., Martín-Azañedo, C., Mosteiro, P., Ancillo, I.,y Poveda, B. (2008). PIC-J. Prueba de Imaginación Creativa para Jóvenes.[Creative Imagination Test for Young Children].Madrid: TEA Ediciones.

Baker-Sennet, J. (1995). Improvisation, planning and the creative process. Infancia y Aprendizaje, 69, 111-126.

Calero, M.D., García-Martín, M.B., y Robles, M.A. (2011). Learning Potential in High IQ Children: The Contribution of Dynamic Assessment to the Identification of Gifted Children. Learning and Individual Differences, 21(2), 176-181.

Corbalán, F.J., Martínez, F., Donolo, D.S., Alonso, C., Tejerina, M., y Limiñana, R.M. (2003). CREA. Inteligencia CreativaUna medida cognitiva de la creatividad. [Creative Intelligence. A cognitive measure of creativity]. Madrid. TEA Ediciones.

Craggs, J.G., Sanchez, J., Kibby, M.Y., Gilger, J.W., y Hynd, G.W.(2006). Brain morphology and neuropsychological profiles in a family displaying dyslexia and superior nonverbal intelligence. Cortex, 42(8),1107–1118.

Dai, D.Y. (2005). Reductionism versus emergentism: a framework for understanding conceptions of giftednees. Roeper Review, 27,144-151.

Das,J.P., y Naglieri, J.A. (1997). Cognitive Assessment System. Chicago, IL: Riverside Publishing.

Das, J.P., Naglieri, J.A., y Kirby, J.R. (1994). Assessment of cognitive processes. Needham & Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.

Das, J.P., Kar, R., y Parrilla, R.K. (1996). Cognitive planning. The psychological basis of intelligent behavior.London: Sage Publications.

Deng, C.P., Liu, M., Wei, W., Chan, R.C., yDas, J.P. (2011). Latent factor structure of the Das-Naglieri Cognitive Assessment System: A confirmatory factor analysis in a Chinese setting. Research inDevelopmental Disabilities, 32(5), 1988–1997.

Dole, S. (2000). The implications of the risk and resilience literature for gifted students with learning disabilities. Roeper Review, 23,91-95.

Hernández, M.E, Speirs, K.L., Burney, V.H., y Cook, A.L. (2014). The Relationship of Cognitive and Executive Functioning With Achievement in Gifted Kindergarten Children. Gifted Child Quarterly, 58, 167-182.

Kaufman, J.C. (2015). Why creativity isn’t in IQ tests, why it matters, and why it won ?t change anytime soon probably. Journal of Intelligence, 3, 59-72.

McCrea, S.M. (2009). A review and empirical study of the composite scales of the Das-Naglieri: Cognitive Assessment System. Psychology Research and Behavior Management, 2,59-79.

Naglieri, J.C. (1999). Essentials of CAS Assessment. New York: Wiley.Naglieri, J.A., yDas, J.P. (1997). Das-Naglieri Cognitive Assessment System.Itasca, IL. Riverside.

Naglieri, J.A., yKaufman, J.C. (2001). Understanding intelligence, giftedness and creativity using the PASS theory. Roeper Review, 23, 151-156.

Nicpon, M.F., Allmon, A., Sieck, B., yStinson, R. (2010). Empirical investigation of twice-exceptionality: where have we been and where are we going? Gifted Child Quarterly, 55,3-17.

Pérez-Álvarez, F., y Timoneda, C. (2007). A Better Look at Intelligent Behavior. Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publishers, Inc.

Pérez-Álvarez, F.,yTimoneda-Gallart, C. (2014).Intelligent behavior and neuroscience: What we know—and don’t know—about how we think. EnT.C. Papadopoulos, R.K. Parrila, yJ.R.Kirby,Cognition, Intelligence, and Achievement: A Tribute to J.P. Das.(pp.419-442). NY Elsevier Inc.

Pérez-Álvarez, F., Pérez-Serra, A., yTimoneda-Gallart, C. (2013). A better look at learning: how does the brain express the mind. Psychology, 4,760-770.

Plucker, J.A. (2001). Looking back, looking around, looking forward: The impact of intelligence theories on gifted education. Roeper Review, 23, 124-125.

Reis, S.M., yMcCoach, D.B. (2000). The underachievement of gifted students. What do we know and where do we go? Gifted Child Quaterly, 44,158-170.

Robinson, S.M. (1999). Meeting the needs of students who are gifted and have learning disabilities. Journal ofLearning Disabilities, 34, 195-204.

Schmader, T.,yJohns, M.(2003). Converging evidence that stereotype threat reduces working memory capacity.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(3),440-452.

Schofield, N.J., yAshman, A.F. (1987). The cognitive processing of gifted, high average and low average ability students. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 57(1), 9-20.

Shore, B.M. (2000). Metacognition and flexibility: Qualitative differences in how gifted children think. In R.C. Friedman, & B.M. Shore (Eds.), Talents unfolding: Cognition and development (pp. 17-187). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Steiner, H.H., yCarr, M. (2003). Cognitive development in gifted children: toward a more precise understanding of emergent differences in intelligence. Educational Psychology Review, 15, 215-246.

Steiner, H.H. (2006). A micro genetic analysis of strategic variability ingifted and average-ability children. Gifted Child Quarterly, 50, 62-74.

Sternberg, R.J., Jarvin, L.,yGrigorenko, E.L. (2011). Exploration in giftedness. New York: Cambridge. University Press.

Swaiman, K.F., Ashwal, S., and Ferriero, D.M. (2006).Pediatric Neurology. Principles & Practice.4th ed. Philadelphia, PA: Mosby Inc.

Yuste, C., Martínez, R., y Galve, J.L. (2002). Batería de aptitudes diferenciales y generales. [Differential and general skills battery]. BADyG-BADyG-M.CEPE. Madrid.

Webb, J.T. (2001). Mis-diagnosis and dual diagnosis of Gifted children: Gifted and LD, ADHD, OCD, oppositional defiant disorder. En N. HafensteinyF. Rayney, (Eds.), Perspectives in gifted education: Twice excepcional children(pp. 23-31). Denver: Ricks Center for Gifted Children, University of Denver.

Wechsler, D. (2003). WISC-IV technical and interpretive manual.San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

Weinfeld, R.,Barnes-Robinson, L.,Jeweler, S., y Shevitz, B. (2002).Academic programs for gifted and talented/learning disabled students. Roeper Review, 24, 226-233.